Encephalitis > Volume 1(2); 2021 > Article
Choi, Kim, La, Son, and Lee: Food memory test to evaluate memory function

Abstract

Purpose

To evaluate cognitive function with ease at bedside, we developed a novel neurologic exam called the “food memory test (FMT)” and evaluated its validity for use in clinical practice.

Methods

In this prospective study in a neurology clinic, we asked patients about what they had eaten for the soup and main dish at the last meal [FMT1] and the second-to-last meal [FMT2]. If they answered correctly for both the soup and main dish, they received a “pass” score. If they did not answer or chose the wrong food, they received a “fail” score. We also performed conventional cognitive function tests for comparison.

Results

A total of 27 patients was enrolled, and 12 (44.4%) passed the FMT1 test. FMT1 has a strong correlation with conventional memory function tests, including time-place orientation, three-word recall, the Seoul Verbal Learning Test, and the Rey Complex Figure Test . FMT1 was not correlated with a depression score or with frontal lobe function tests. FMT2 showed less significant correlation with conventional memory tests.

Conclusion

These results suggest that FMT1 is a reliable bedside test to evaluate recent memory. Clinical application of FMT in daily clinical practice is warranted.

Introduction

Cognitive function tests evaluate an individual’s memory, attention, frontal lobe/executive function, and language-related functions [1,2]. Therefore, they structuralize overall cognitive function and help with a subject’s diagnosis and anticipated prognosis [3,4]. Common memory function tests include sub-questions of the Mini-Mental State Examination, the verbal learning test (VLT), and the Rey Complex Figure Test (RCFT) [5,6]. However, they are sophisticated and time consuming, and it is difficult to perform them quickly at bedside or in an outpatient clinic. In addition, since conventional memory function tests use memory registration of predefined items, daily repetitive testing to assess improvement is difficult due to the learning curve effect. Furthermore, conventional memory tests can be biased by attention deficit, depressive mood, or pseudodementia [7,8].
Clinicians have used the food memory test (FMT) to evaluate patient memory function empirically, but this function has never been studied structurally. Since people attend to what they eat during a meal, the attention factor can be minimized in this test. However, FMT is uncomplicated, especially in Korea, as many people eat soup and a main dish for each meal.
We tested the FMT for reliability and usability as well as whether it could be substituted as a conventional memory test. In this study, subjects needing the memory function test in the neurology clinic were enrolled for FMT along with a correlation analysis and a conventional cognitive function test.

Methods

We prospectively performed a FMT and a standard cognitive function test set (Seoul Neuropsychological Screening Battery, SNSB) [1,2] in patients admitted to or visiting the neurology clinic at Seoul National University Hospital for a cognitive function evaluation. The time from onset of cognitive issues to the cognitive function test was not controlled because tests should be applicable and reliable at any time point regardless of disease phase. This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Seoul National University Hospital, South Korea (No. H-1912-078-1088). All participants or their legal representatives provided written informed consent.
For the FMT, we asked each patient what they ate as a soup and main dish at the last meal (FMT1) and the second-to-last meal (FMT2), a total of four questions. We collected information about the food consumed from the hospital diet data or from caregivers. If they answered the correct menu (soup and main dish) in one meal, we scored “pass,” assigning them to the FMT-pass group. If they did not answer or indicated the wrong food, we scored “fail,” assigning them to the FMT-fail group. If the patient had only one of soup or main dish, we used the single food to score pass or fail.
We performed SNSB using a standardized scoring scale [9]. Among the items in the SNSB, some were used for a comparison analysis with the FMT; the Korean version of the Mini-Mental State Exam (K-MMSE), the Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS), the Seoul Verbal Learning Test (SVLT; immediate recall, delayed recall, and recognition), the RCFT (immediate recall and delayed recall), the contrasting program, the Go/No-go test, the fist-edge-palm, alternating hand movement, alternating square and triangle, the Luria loop, the Controlled Oral Word Association Test, spontaneous speech, comprehension, repetition, finger naming, right-left orientation, body part identification, calculation, the praxis test, and the digit span test.
Patients were categorized by diagnosis, location of brain lesion, and medications. We categorized diagnosis as autoimmune encephalitis, infectious diseases, tumors, degenerative diseases, or normal. The location of the brain lesion was divided into dominant hemisphere, temporal lobe, frontal lobe, parietal lobe, and Papez circuit (hippocampal formation, entorhinal cortex, fornix, mammillary bodies, anterior thalamus, and cingulum) [10]. Medications were categorized into antiepileptic drugs, antipsychotics, antidepressants, and sedatives. Sedatives were defined as central nervous system (CNS) depressants, such as barbiturates and benzodiazepines. Brain lesion location and drug categories could overlap if there were multiple lesions or medications.
Data were analyzed with IBM SPSS version 25.0 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA) and were expressed as mean ± standard deviation. The Student t-test or the Mann-Whitney U-test was used for independent data. For categorical variables, Fisher exact test (np < 5 or n [1 – p] < 5) or Pearson chi-square test (np ≥ 5 and n [1 – p] ≥ 5) was used for independent data. The independent t-test or Fisher exact test was used to compare data, and a p-value of <0.05 was considered significant. Binary logistic regression was used to identify key independent variables.

Results

Clinical characteristics of patients

We enrolled a total of 27 patients, including 15 males and 12 females, with a mean age of 50.4 ± 22.2 years (range, 18–83 years) (Table 1). Among them, 16 patients had autoimmune encephalitis, four had CNS infection (one intracranial tuberculoma, one herpes simplex virus (HSV)-1 encephalitis, one HSV-2 encephalitis, and one HSV encephalitis), two had tumors (two primary CNS lymphomas), four had degenerative diseases (three dementia and one hydrocephalus), and one was a normal volunteer. Of those with brain lesions, 17 had lesion in the dominant hemisphere, 14 in the temporal lobe, 10 in the frontal lobe, four in the parietal lobe, and four in the Papez circuit. Medications were also categorized; 16 used antiepileptic drugs, three used antipsychotics, three used sedatives, and one used antidepressants.
Among the 27 patients, 12 passed the FMT1 (44.4%) and nine passed the FMT2 (33.3%). When comparing the differences of clinical characteristics between the FMT1-pass and FMT1-fail groups, there was no variation in age, sex, diagnosis, or medication profiles. However, the FMT1-fail group had a higher frequency of non-frontal brain lesions (p = 0.004). When comparing clinical characteristics between the FMT2-pass and FMT2-fail groups, there was no difference in age, sex, diagnosis, medication profiles, and brain lesions (Table 1).

FMT1 predicted the conventional memory function test score

We analyzed correlation and reliability of FMT1 to the conventional cognitive function tests. Meal-to-test time (MTT) and modified Rankin scale were consistent between the FMT1/FMT2-pass group and the FMT1/FMT2-fail group. In K-MMSE subitems, the FMT1-pass group showed higher scores compared with the FMT1-fail group in time-place orientation (FMT1-pass, 9.2 ± 2.0 and FMT1-fail, 6.5 ± 3.4; p = 0.016) with three-word recall tests (2.6 ± 0.9 and 1.5 ± 1.3, p = 0.015). In SVLT, the FMT1-pass group showed higher scores than the FMT1-fail group in immediate recall (FMT1-pass, 20.4 ± 7.5 and FMT1-fail, 13.0 ± 3.8; p = 0.007), delayed recall (6.3 ± 3.9 and 1.3 ± 1.7, p = 0.001), and recognition (20.5 ± 3.8 and 16.6 ± 2.8, p = 0.006) (Figure 1AC). In the RCFT, the FMT1-pass group showed higher scores than the FMT1-fail group in immediate recall (FMT1-pass, 16.3 ± 11.5 and FMT1-fail, 4.0 ± 6.3; p = 0.006) and delayed recall (15.7 ± 10.6 and 3.5 ± 6.0, p = 0.004) (Figure 1D, E). No tests evaluated attention, frontal, or executive function; however, language and related function showed a significant difference between the FMT1-pass and FMT1-fail groups, except in right-left orientation. GDS showed no significant difference between the FMT1-pass group and FMT1-fail group.
We also analyzed the correlation and reliability of FMT2 to the conventional cognitive function tests. The FMT2-pass group showed a significant difference from the FMT2-fail group, but only in time-place orientation, three-word recall, SVLT immediate recall, SVLT delayed recall, and the contrasting program (Table 2).
We further determined the key domains in the conventional cognitive function test, predicting FMT1 performance using logistic regression analysis. While other SNSB categories showed a nonsignificant association with FMT1 results, SVLT delayed recall score was significant (p = 0.010), and the odds ratio was 1.745, suggesting that the FMT1 test reflects recent memory function.

Discussion

We showed that FMT1 is a reliable memory test that can predict the results of conventional memory tests. FMT1 was not influenced by attention, frontal lobe function tests, or depression. This indicates that FMT can be conducted in environments where patients find it difficult to focus on the test itself, such as the emergency room, and in patients with attention deficit due to depression including those with pseudodementia. FMT1 was not affected by MTT, so it can be performed at any time within a range of 1 to 6 hours after the last meal. FMT1-fail patients had more non-frontal lesions, which could be because FMT1-fail patients had greater involvement of memory-related brain structures outside of the frontal lobe. In the same context, the FMT1-fail group showed more frequent failure in the right-left orientation test, which is localized to the left parietal lobe [11]. FMT1 was more reliable than FMT2, at least in correlation with conventional cognitive function tests.
FMT1 is more likely to represent the recent verbal and episodic memory. This is supported by the finding that SVLT delayed recall predicts FMT1 performance. The delayed recall of the VLT is a sensitive measure for diagnosis of mild amnestic cognitive impairment and early Alzheimer disease [12]. While language functions were not different in the FMT1-pass and fail groups in the current study, patients who had naming difficulties or loss of semantic knowledge about food could not perform the test. Furthermore, patients with language problems could also not perform the conventional memory function test, while some patients with hand weakness could not perform the RCFT, as experienced in our testing (Table 2).
We also showed that FMT1 is a reliable test that predicts conventional memory tests. Since the number of patients and number of disease categories are limited, we must collect more data and conduct this test in a larger population. Since our patients were mainly enrolled in the autoimmune neurology clinic, the test must be reproduced in various disease categories. In addition, because food memory can be biased by variability and novelty of meals, the viability of this test with other ethnicities, or with different food cultures, must be validated separately. Moreover, hospital meals for admitted patients have limited variability. This study could not confirm intra- and inter-rater reliability due to difficulties in repeating the FMT on the same patient. Still, we believe that the FMT is useful for daily clinical practice in neurology.
In conclusion, FMT1-pass or fail reflected key memory function tests, similar to the delayed recall of the VLT. Furthermore, the FMT1 score was not biased by depression (pseudodementia) or attention deficit, rendering it more reliable. Because the FMT1 takes only several seconds to complete, we suggest it as a quick, simple, bedside-compatible memory test that warrants further studies.

Notes

Conflicts of Interest

Soon-Tae Lee has been editorial board of encephalitis since October 2020. He was not involved in the review process of this original article.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, Supervision, Funding acquisition: ST Lee; Data collection, Interpretation: JH Choi, DW Kim, HJ La; Project administration: H Son; Writing-original draft: JH Choi, DW Kim, HJ La; Writing-review & editing: ST Lee.

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by Student Research Program of Seoul National University College of Medicine (2019).

Figure 1.

Correlation between FMT1 and conventional memory tests

Correlation with the immediate score (A), delayed score (B), and recognition score (C) of Seoul Verbal Learning Test (SVLT); correlation with the immediate score (D) and delayed score (E) of Rey Complex Figure Test (RCFT). Whiskers indicate the range of scores. ***p < 0.001, **p < 0.01.
FMT1, food memory test for the last meal.
encephalitis-2021-00031f1.jpg
Table 1
Demographic and clinical characteristics of patients
Characteristic Total FMT1
FMT2
Pass Fail p-value Pass Fail p-value
No. of patients 27 12 15 9 18
Age (yr) 50.4 ± 22.2 45.1 ± 24.0 54.7 ± 21.4 0.280 44.7 ± 24.3 53.3 ± 22.0 0.359
Male sex 15 (55.6) 7 (58.3) 8 (53.3) 0.795 5 (55.6) 10 (55.6) >0.999
Diagnosisa)
 Autoimmune encephalitis 16 (59.3) 7 (58.3) 9 (60.0) 0.930 7 (77.8) 9 (50.0) 0.231
 CNS infection 4 (14.8) 2 (16.7) 2 (13.3) >0.999 1 (11.1) 3 (16.7) >0.999
 Tumor 2 (7.4) 1 (8.3) 1 (6.7) >0.999 0 (0) 2 (11.1) 0.538
 Degenerative disease 4 (14.8) 1 (8.3) 3 (20.0) 0.605 0 (0) 4 (22.2) 0.268
 Normal 1 (3.7) 1 (8.3) 0 (0) 0.444 1 (11.1) 0 (0) 0.333
MRI lesion
 Dominant hemisphereb) 17 (63.0) 7 (58.3) 10 (66.7) 0.656 6 (66.7) 11 (61.1) 0.778
 Temporal lobe 14 (51.9) 5 (41.7) 9 (60.0) 0.343 3 (33.3) 11 (61.1) 0.173
 Frontal lobe 10 (37.0) 8 (66.7) 2 (13.3) 0.004 5 (55.6) 5 (27.8) 0.219
 Parietal lobe 4 (14.8) 3 (25.0) 1 (6.7) 0.294 3 (33.3) 1 (5.6) 0.093
 Papez circuit 4 (14.8) 2 (16.7) 2 (13.3) >0.999 2 (22.2) 2 (11.1) 0.582
Drug
 Antiepileptic drugs 16 (59.3) 6 (50.0) 10 (66.7) 0.381 6 (66.7) 10 (55.6) 0.580
 Antipsychotics 3 (11.1) 0 (0) 3 (20.0) 0.231 2 (22.2) 1 (5.6) 0.250
 Antidepressants 1 (3.7) 0 (0) 1 (6.7) >0.999 0 (0) 1 (5.6) >0.999
 Sedatives 3 (11.1) 0 (0) 3 (20.0) 0.231 1 (11.1) 2 (11.1) >0.999

Values are presented as number only, mean ± standard deviation, or number (%).

FMT, food memory test; FMT1, FMT for the last meal; FMT2, FMT for the second-to-last meal; CNS, central nervous system; MRI, magnetic resonance imaging.

a)CNS infection includes intracranial tuberculoma and viral encephalitis. Degenerative diseases include dementia, communicating hydrocephalus, and Alzheimer disease.

b)Dominant hemisphere is the one opposite the dominant hand.

Table 2
Food memory test and patient Seoul Neuropsychological Screening Battery Scores
Characteristic Total FMT1
FMT2
Pass Fail p-value Pass Fail p-value
No. of patients 27 12 15 9 18
MTT (hr)
 First 2.8 ± 1.2 2.6 ± 1.6 2.9 ± 0.9 0.609 2.2 ± 1.3 3.1 ± 1.1 0.080
 Second 12.1 ± 5.2 14.2 ± 4.3 10.1 ± 5.4 0.045 12.7 ± 5.0 11.7 ± 5.5 0.674
 mRS 3 (0–4) 2.5 (0–4) 3 (2–4) 0.110 3 (0–4) 3 (2–4) 0.891
K-MMSE
 Time-place orientation 7.7 ± 3.1 9.2 ± 2.0 6.5 ± 3.4 0.016 9.6 ± 0.5 6.7 ± 3.4 0.003
 Three-word registration 2.9 ± 0.3 2.9 ± 0.3 2.9 ± 0.4 0.695 3.0 ± 0.0 2.8 ± 0.4 0.083
 Three-word recall 2.0 ± 1.3 2.6 ± 0.9 1.5 ± 1.3 0.015 2.7 ± 0.7 1.6 ± 1.4 0.013
 Language 6.9 ± 1.6 7.3 ± 0.9 6.5 ± 1.9 0.189 7.0 ± 1.2 6.8 ± 1.7 0.798
 Total 23.1 ± 6.2 25.4 ± 4.6 21.3 ± 6.7 0.081 25.4 ± 3.6 21.9 ± 6.9 0.095
SVLTa)
 Immediate recall 16.3 ± 6.8 20.4 ± 7.5 13.0 ± 3.8 0.007 20.3 ± 7.8 14.3 ± 5.3 0.026
 Delayed recall 3.5 ± 3.8 6.3 ± 3.9 1.3 ± 1.7 0.001 6.1 ± 4.0 2.2 ± 3.1 0.010
 Recognition 18.4 ± 3.8 20.5 ± 3.8 16.6 ± 2.8 0.006 19.8 ± 4.0 17.7 ± 3.6 0.177
RCFTb)
 Immediate recall 9.4 ± 10.8 16.3 ± 11.5 4.0 ± 6.3 0.006 15.0 ± 10.7 6.8 ± 10.0 0.074
 Delayed recall 8.9 ± 10.2 15.7 ± 10.6 3.5 ± 6.0 0.004 14.0 ± 10.3 6.5 ± 9.5 0.085
Attention
 Digit span: forward 5.3 ± 2.1 6.0 ± 1.9 4.7 ± 2.2 0.292 5.6 ± 2.2 5.2 ± 2.1 0.835
 Digit span: backward 3.6 ± 1.6 2.9 ± 1.3 2.9 ± 1.3 0.256 4.0 ± 1.7 3.3 ± 1.6 0.466
Language and related functions
 Comprehension 20 (74.1) 11 (91.7) 9 (60.0) 0.062 8 (88.9) 12 (66.7) 0.214
 Repetition 13.7 ± 1.9 14.2 ± 0.9 13.4 ± 2.4 0.256 14.3 ± 1.1 13.4 ± 2.1 0.239
 Finger naming 24 (88.9) 11 (91.7) 13 (86.7) 0.681 9 (100) 15 (83.3) 0.194
 Right-left orientation 20 (74.1) 12 (100) 8 (53.3) 0.008 9 (100) 11 (61.1) 0.059
 Body part identification 20 (74.1) 10 (83.3) 10 (66.7) 0.326 7 (77.8) 13 (72.2) 0.756
 Calculation 8.8 ± 3.8 9.3 ± 3.7 8.4 ± 4.0 0.540 9.4 ± 3.4 8.5 ± 4.9 0.557
 Praxis 26 (96.3) 11 (91.7) 15 (100) 0.255 9 (100) 17 (94.4) 0.471
 Ideomotor 4.1 ± 1.2 4.6 ± 0.9 3.7 ± 1.2 0.148 4.4 ± 1.0 3.9 ± 1.2 0.578
Frontal/executive function
 Contrasting program 16.9 ± 6.6 18.3 ± 5.8 15.7 ± 7.1 0.317 19.7 ± 0.7 15.5 ± 7.7 0.036
 Go/No-go test 14.8 ± 7.9 16.8 ± 6.8 13.1 ± 8.6 0.233 15.9 ± 7.4 14.2 ± 8.3 0.614
 Fist-edge-palm 21 (77.8) 10 (83.3) 11 (73.3) 0.535 8 (88.9) 13 (72.2) 0.326
 Alternating hand movement 22 (81.5) 10 (83.3) 12 (80.0) 0.446 8 (88.9) 14 (77.8) 0.147
 Alternating square and triangle 17 (63.0) 7 (58.3) 10 (66.7) 0.678 6 (66.7) 11 (61.1) 0.607
 Luria loop 18 (66.7) 8 (66.7) 10 (66.7) 0.943 6 (66.7) 12 (66.7) 0.819
COWATc)
 Animal 10.7 ± 5.4 12.6 ± 6.2 9.2 ± 4.7 0.109 12.2 ± 5.5 9.9 ± 5.4 0.313
 Supermarket 9.2 ± 5.0 11.4 ± 4.6 7.4 ± 4.6 0.030 10.3 ± 4.9 8.6 ± 5.0 0.405
 Phonemic sum 19.4 ± 13.4 23.7 ± 14.4 15.6 ± 11.8 0.132 23.4 ± 15.6 17.2 ± 11.8 0.267
GDS 12.9 ± 8.1 13.1 ± 8.2 12.7 ± 8.4 0.898 10.3 ± 8.4 14.1 ± 7.9 0.262

Values are presented as number only, mean ± standard deviation, or number (%).

FMT, food memory test; FMT1, FMT for the last meal; FMT2, FMT for the second-to-last meal; MTT, meal-to-test time; mRS, modified Rankin scale; K-MMSE, Korean version of the Mini-Mental State Examination; SVLT, Seoul Verbal Learning Test; RCFT, Rey Complex Figure Test; COWAT, Controlled Oral Word Association Test; GDS, Geriatric Depression Scale.

a)One patient in the FMT1-fail group ended the test halfway through the SVLT Recognition exam. Analysis excluded the missing data.

b)One patient in the FMT1- and FMT2-pass group could not use a pencil due to hemiplegia. Another patient in the FMT1- and FMT2-fail group could not perform the exam due to immobility. These patients could not perform RCFT immediately or the RCFT delayed-recall test. Analysis excluded missing data.

c)Due to language barriers, one patient in the FMT1- and FMT2-fail group could not perform the COWAT phonemic test. Analysis excluded the missing data.

References

1. Kang Y, Na DL. Seoul Neuropsychological Screening Battery. Incheon: Human Brain Research & Consulting Co.; 2003.
2. Kang Y, Jahng S, Na DL. Seoul Neuropsychological Screening Battery, 2nd ed (SNSB-II). Seoul: Human Brain Research & Consulting Co.; 2012.
3. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 1975;12:189–198.
crossref pmid
4. Jansen WJ, Handels RL, Visser PJ, et al. The diagnostic and prognostic value of neuropsychological assessment in memory clinic patients. J Alzheimers Dis 2017;55:679–689.
crossref pmid
5. Velayudhan L, Ryu SH, Raczek M, et al. Review of brief cognitive tests for patients with suspected dementia. Int Psychogeriatr 2014;26:1247–1262.
crossref pmid pmc
6. Egloff L, Studerus E, Zimmermann R, et al. Evaluating verbal learning and memory in patients with an at-risk mental state or first episode psychosis using structural equation modelling. PLoS One 2018;13:e0196936.
crossref pmid pmc
7. Everaert J, Duyck W, Koster EH. Attention, interpretation, and memory biases in subclinical depression: a proof-of-principle test of the combined cognitive biases hypothesis. Emotion 2014;14:331–340.
crossref pmid
8. Small GW, Liston EH, Jarvik LF. Diagnosis and treatment of dementia in the aged. West J Med 1981;135:469–481.
pmid pmc
9. Ahn HJ, Chin J, Park A, et al. Seoul Neuropsychological Screening Battery-dementia version (SNSB-D): a useful tool for assessing and monitoring cognitive impairments in dementia patients. J Korean Med Sci 2010;25:1071–1076.
crossref pmid pmc
10. Shah A, Jhawar SS, Goel A. Analysis of the anatomy of the Papez circuit and adjoining limbic system by fiber dissection techniques. J Clin Neurosci 2012;19:289–298.
crossref pmid
11. Chen TY, Chen CY, Yen CH, et al. Acute parietal lobe infarction presenting as Gerstmann's syndrome and cognitive decline mimicking senile dementia. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat 2013;9:937–940.
crossref pmid pmc
12. Zhao Q, Lv Y, Zhou Y, Hong Z, Guo Q. Short-term delayed recall of auditory verbal learning test is equivalent to long-term delayed recall for identifying amnestic mild cognitive impairment. PLoS One 2012;7:e51157.
crossref pmid pmc
TOOLS
METRICS Graph View
  • 0 Crossref
  • 0  
  • 5,756 View
  • 78 Download
ORCID iDs

Soon-Tae Lee
https://orcid.org/0000-0003-4767-7564

Related articles


ABOUT
BROWSE ARTICLES
EDITORIAL POLICY
FOR CONTRIBUTORS
Editorial Office
101, Daehak-ro, Jongno-gu, Seoul 03080, Republic of Korea​
Tel: +82-2-2072-0629    Fax: +82-2-765-7920    E-mail: editor@encephalitisjournal.org                

Copyright © 2024 by Korean Encephalitis and Neuroinflammation Society.

Developed in M2PI

Close layer
prev next
Close layer