Changes in laboratory mice after observation of deceased conspecifics: a pilot suicidality study in animals
Article information
Abstract
Purpose
Suicidality can be a serious feature of psychiatric symptoms in encephalitis. Investigating the psychiatric behavior associated with suicidality in animal models of encephalitis is important; thus, determining whether normal laboratory animals are aware of death is necessary.
Methods
To examine the behavioral and brain activity changes associated with death of conspecifics, laboratory mice were exposed to a cadaveric mouse or an anesthetized mouse. Behavioral tasks associated with anxiety and locomotion were conducted after repeated exposure. Neural activity in the medial prefrontal cortex during the cadaver exploration was investigated using electroencephalographic recordings.
Results
During repeated exposure, mice in the cadaver group showed a gradual decrease in time exploring the cadaver, which was not observed in mice in the anesthesia group. The cadaver group also exhibited increased levels of anxiety in the light/dark transition and elevated plus maze tasks and displayed increased locomotor activity in the open field test. In an electrophysiological study, different brain oscillations were observed when mice were exposed to a cadaveric mouse and an anesthetized mouse. Enhanced delta-band activity and reduced theta- and alpha-band activities were observed during cadaver exploration.
Conclusion
The present study results showed that experiences involving dead conspecifics strongly affect mouse behavior and brain activity. These findings may be helpful in treating patients with psychiatric symptoms and aid in understanding the concept of death recognition/awareness in laboratory animals.
Introduction
Encephalitis can often be accompanied by emotional and psychological problems, such as anxiety, depression, and mood swings. In antibody-mediated autoimmune encephalitis, such as anti-N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor encephalitis, the degree of psychiatric symptoms is severe. In addition, suicidality can be a serious feature of psychiatric symptoms in encephalitis [1]. Therefore, studying suicidality in animals can be helpful in treating patients with psychiatric brain disorders.
Many nonhuman mammalian species show unique responses to dead conspecifics, such as remaining next to the deceased conspecific, vocalizations, grooming, licking, or carrying the body [2-6]. To date, whether these behavioral observations are relevant to human behaviors regarding death, including death-related psychological states (e.g., grief, mourning, bereavement, and lamentation) and fear or anxiety of mortality remains unclear. However, animals presumably exhibit such behaviors because they can distinguish between life and death and recognize death as something other than living [3,7-10].
In the wild, animals may occasionally observe dying conspecifics or may encounter dead conspecifics due to events such as aging, disease, or enemy attack. This experience may be traumatic as the animals come to recognize or become aware of death. However, animals that live in a laboratory cannot have such observational experiences involving dead bodies of their conspecifics owing to animal welfare practices and the control of experiments in accordance with the guidelines of the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee [11]. Thus, whether laboratory animals can discriminate between living and dead conspecifics, and between life and death, remains unclear. If laboratory animals can process this distinction, their responses and the importance of these experiences remain unclear. Answers to these questions may provide basic information regarding animal thanatology and for suicidality studies in animals as well as help elucidate the evolutionary origins of death-related behaviors.
Mice are commonly used experimental animals. They are highly social and even display empathy-like behaviors toward each other [12-15]. Thus, mouse models can be useful for assessing the neurobiological correlates of death recognition. In the present pilot suicidality study, laboratory animals were used to determine how a dead body (cadaver) of a conspecific affected mouse behavior and behavioral changes were compared with those elicited using anesthetized mice. Furthermore, neural activity in the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) during cadaver exploration was investigated using electrophysiological assessment in vivo.
Methods
Animals and treatments
Male C57BL/6 mice were used in the present study. Animals were maintained with free access to food and water under a 12-hour light/dark cycle. All experiments were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee in Seoul National University Hospital (SNUH-IACUC, No. 14-0210-S1A1) and animals were maintained in the facility accredited AAALAC International (#001169) in accordance with Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals 8th edition (National Research Council 2011). All efforts were made to minimize suffering. The overall study consisted of two major experiments shown in Figure 1A. Mice 10–12 weeks old were used as subject mice and divided into two groups: (1) cadaver group, exposed to a cadaveric conspecific; (2) anesthesia group, exposed to an anesthetized conspecific. Age-matched male C57BL/6 mice that had been used for other behavioral experiments and were scheduled to be sacrificed were used as cadaveric or anesthetized mice. The mice scheduled to be used as cadaveric mice were sacrificed using CO2 immediately prior to each experiment. The mice scheduled to be used as anesthetized mice were deeply anesthetized with an intraperitoneal injection of ketamine (70 mg/kg) and xylazine hydrochloride (10 mg/kg) immediately prior to each experiment.
Social investigation of a cadaver and an anesthetized mouse target
The experiment was performed as previously described [16]. A single subject mouse was allowed to roam freely in a clean cage (22 × 19 × 17 cm, 3-cm sawdust layer) for 10 minutes. The cages used were identical to cages in which the mice were normally housed. After the 10-minute period, a cadaveric or anesthetized male mouse (target mouse) was placed in the middle of the cage. Then, the subject mouse was allowed to roam freely for 15 minutes (test session). Behavior toward the target mouse was video-recorded for 15 minutes and the duration of contact (sniffing, touching, and climbing) were measured and analyzed for 5 or 10 minutes. A mouse was considered to be sniffing the target mouse when its head was facing the target mouse within 1 inch. The cage, target mouse, and sawdust were changed for every subject mouse. This experiment was repeatedly performed once a day for 3 days.
Elevated plus maze and light/dark transition
For assessment of anxiety, elevated plus maze and light/dark transition tasks were performed as previously described [13,16,17]. The elevated plus maze was made of plastic and consisted of two white open arms (25 × 8 cm), two black enclosed arms (25 × 8 × 20 cm), and a central platform (8 × 8 × 8 cm) in the form of a cross. The maze was placed 50 cm above the floor. Mice were individually placed in the center with their heads directed toward one of the closed arms. The total time spent in each arm or in the center was analyzed by video monitoring for 5 minutes. An arm entry was defined and counted when all four paws crossed from the center into an arm and was used for measuring the amount of time spent in each arm.
For the light/dark transition task, a light/dark box (30 × 45 × 27 cm) made of plastic comprised of a dark compartment (one third of the total area) and light compartment with a hole in the middle. The light compartment was illuminated at 400 lux. The elapsed time to entry (all four paws) into the light compartment (latency) and the amount of time (duration) spent in each compartment were measured over a 5-minute period by video monitoring.
Open field test
To assess locomotor activity, the open field test was performed as previously described [13,18]. The open field box was made of white plastic (40 × 40 × 40 cm). Individual mice were placed in the periphery of the field and the paths of the animals recorded with a video camera. The total distance traveled for 10 minutes was analyzed using a software program (EthoVision XT; Noldus Information Technology, Wageningen, The Netherlands).
In vivo electrophysiology for electroencephalographic
Electroencephalographic (EEG) surgery and recording in vivo were performed as previously described [13,18,19]. For EEG monitoring during a social behavioral task, nine naïve mice were subjected to EEG surgery 1 week before the experiment. For surgery, the animals were anesthetized with an intraperitoneal injection of ketamine (90 mg/kg) and xylazine hydrochloride (40 mg/kg). Electrode implantation was performed using a stereotaxic apparatus (Kopf Instruments, Tujunga, CA, USA). EEG recordings were obtained with tungsten electrodes (0.005 in, 2 MΩ), positioned in the mPFC (anterior-posterior, −1.94 to –1.70 mm; medial-lateral, 0.25–0.45 mm; and dorsal-ventral, 2.75 mm) from the bregma with grounding over the cerebellum. The electrodes were fixed to the skull with cyanoacrylate adhesive and dental acrylic cement. After a 1-week recovery from brain surgery, animals were placed in a small acrylic cage (15 × 20 × 15 cm) and allowed to move freely in the cage. EEG recordings combined with video monitoring were performed simultaneously during the behavioral task on social investigation of a cadaveric conspecific or an anesthetized conspecific. Subject mice were sequentially exposed to anesthetized or cadaveric conspecifics at 15-minute intervals. The order of exposure to targets was random. Two epochs (10 seconds each) of EEG signals during exploration of the conspecific by each mouse, from 1 to 3 minutes after the start of the behavioral task, were randomly selected and used for analysis. The five EEG frequency bands, delta (1.5–4 Hz), theta (4–8 Hz), alpha (8–12 Hz), beta (12–30 Hz), and gamma (30–60 Hz), were analyzed for EEG power. The relative normalized power at each individual frequency was presented as a fraction of the sum of powers at all frequencies. The electrical activities were recorded after amplification (×12,000), bandpass filtering from 0.1 to 100 Hz, and digitization at a 400 Hz sampling rate (AS 40) with a digital electroencephalography system (Comet XL; Astro-Med, Inc., Warwick, RI, USA). EEG-video data obtained were analyzed offline using PSG Twin (Astro-Med) and Clampfit (Axon Instruments, Foster City, CA, USA).
Statistical analysis
All data are presented as means ± standard error of the mean. Analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to conduct multiple comparisons of means. Student t-test was performed to determine statistical differences between two means. A p-value < 0.05 was considered statistically significant.
Results
Reduced social investigation of cadaver
To determine how laboratory mice are affected by a dead conspecific, mice were first allowed to explore a dead conspecific (cadaver). The social investigative activity was assessed as duration of contact with the target (a cadaver or an anesthetized mouse). On the first day, the cadaver (exposed to a cadaveric conspecific) (n = 17; 147.88 ± 15.65 seconds over 5 minutes, Figure 1B; 232.93 ± 33.04 seconds over 10 minutes, Figure 1C) and anesthesia groups (exposed to an anesthetized conspecific) (n = 13; 145.32 ± 9.56 seconds over 5 minutes, Figure 1B; 199.45 ± 19.56 seconds over 10 minutes, Figure 1C) spent a similar amount of time in contact with their targets. However, on the second day, the cadaver group (114.72 ± 11.96 seconds over 5 minutes, Figure 1B; 170.08 ± 21.46 seconds over 10 minutes, Figure 1C) showed a significantly shorter contact duration than the anesthesia group (170.85 ± 21.89 seconds over 5 minutes, p < 0.05, Student t-test, Figure 1B; 301.20 ± 42.81 seconds over 10 minutes, p < 0.01, Student t-test, Figure 1C). Similarly, on the third day, the cadaver group (76.92 ± 10.78 seconds over 5 minutes, Figure 1B; 114.85 ± 15.60 seconds over 10 minutes, Figure 1C) showed a significantly shorter contact duration than the anesthesia group (116.25 ± 15.52 seconds over 5 minutes, p < 0.05, Student t-test; Figure 1B; 178.78 ± 19.15 seconds over 10 minutes, p < 0.05, Student t-test; Figure 1C). Notably, the contact duration in the cadaver group gradually became shorter from day 1 to day 3 (F2, 48 = 7.50, p < 0.01, one-way ANOVA, Figure 1B; F2, 48 = 5.83, p < 0.01, one-way ANOVA, Figure 1C); however, the anesthesia group showed no difference (F2, 36 = 2.85, p = 0.07, one-way ANOVA; Figure 1B) or increase on day 2 (F2, 36 = 4.85, p < 0.05, one-way ANOVA; Figure 1C). The results indicated that exploratory activities differed depending on social targets and mice respond to a dead conspecific in a manner different from their response to a living anesthetized conspecific.
Cadaver group showed increased anxiety levels and locomotor activity
After the 3-day exposure experiment, the cadaver and anesthesia groups were subjected to elevated plus maze, light/dark transition, and open field tests to assess anxiety and locomotion. In the elevated plus maze task (Figure 2A), both the cadaver (n = 17, 10.00 ± 2.78 seconds) and anesthesia groups (n = 13, 7.35 ± 2.49 seconds) spent less time in the open arms than the control group (n = 17, 24.62 ± 3.02 seconds, F2, 43 = 11.10, p < 0.001, one-way ANOVA). In addition, the cadaver (251.96 ± 6.54 seconds) and anesthesia groups (261.72 ± 4.56 seconds) spent more time in the closed arms than the control group (229.84 ± 5.91 seconds, F2, 43 = 7.67, p < 0.01, one-way ANOVA). Significant differences were not observed between the cadaver and anesthesia groups.
In the light/dark transition task (Figure 2B), the cadaver group (n = 16, 165.80 ± 25.10 seconds) displayed a longer latency in the light compartment compared with the control group (n = 17, 93.09 ± 16.74 seconds, p < 0.05, Student t-test). Furthermore, the cadaver group displayed longer and shorter amounts of time in the dark (cadaver, 257.93 ± 10.78 seconds; control, 226.03 ± 7.64 seconds, p < 0.05, Student t-test) and light compartments (cadaver, 42.07 ± 10.38 seconds; control, 73.97 ± 7.64 seconds, p < 0.05, Student t-test), respectively, compared with the control group. However, the anesthesia group spent a similar amount of time in each compartment compared with the control group.
To assess locomotor activity, an open field test was performed. Mice in the cadaver group traveled a greater distance (n = 15, 3,758.78 ± 106.85 cm) than mice in the control group (n = 17, 3,323.42 ± 95.95 cm, p < 0.01, Student t-test). However, mice in the anesthesia group (n = 13, 3,395.06 ± 90.06 cm) traveled a distance similar to mice in the control group.
Taken together, these results demonstrated that mice exposed to a cadaveric conspecific had increased levels of anxiety and locomotor activity compared with naïve control mice or mice exposed to an anesthetized conspecific.
Differences in brain oscillations in the medial prefrontal cortex during exploration of cadaveric and anesthetized mice
The mPFC has an important role in social cognitive behavior, including novelty detection and fear recognition [20-26]. EEG signals were measured from the mPFC while mice investigated cadaveric and anesthetized conspecifics (Figure 3A) and brain oscillations compared using a frequency-wise power spectrum analysis. During exploration of a cadaver, subject mice showed significantly enhanced delta-band activity and reduced theta- and alpha-band activities in the mPFC relative to their activities during exploration of an anesthetized conspecific (p < 0.05, Student t-test; Figure 3B). Normalized power spectrum analysis of another EEG signal epoch also showed a similar result (Figure 3C). The results indicated that mice showed different brain activities toward cadaveric and anesthetized conspecifics and can distinguish live from dead bodies.
Discussion
In the wild, animals may frequently encounter dead bodies and observe the death of their family members. These experiences, together with education from family or social groups, may help animals understand or become aware of death and may cause them to exhibit specific behavioral responses to the dead (or to react differently to dead bodies), such as remaining near or carrying the deceased conspecific, as well as vocalizations, grooming, or licking [2-6].
Death-related behavior in primates has been reported in several studies [27-31]. For example, a daughter chimpanzee was reported to groom and stay with her mother’s body, and to sleep fitfully, for a few days after her mother’s death [32]. In addition, elephants reportedly have a generalized response to a dead body or death [33-35]. Elephants gather around a dead conspecific, manipulate the body with their feet and trunks, and often stand vigil for several days, which may indicate their awareness of death [33]. Numerous reports have been published of death-related behavior in other animals, such as giraffes [36], dogs [37], and whales or dolphins [38,39]. Thus, animals, specifically social mammalian species, can distinguish between life and death; however, what a dead body signifies to these animals is unknown.
In the present study, the effects of a dead body on a laboratory mouse that has never previously encountered a dead conspecific and likely has not received any education regarding death from its family (parents or cagemates) were investigated. Exposure to a cadaveric conspecific rendered mice more anxious than exposure to an anesthetized conspecific. In the social investigation experiment, differences were not observed in contact time between the cadaver and anesthesia groups on the first day; however, differences were observed on the second and third days. If mice had an innate fear of dead bodies, mice exposed to a cadaveric conspecific should have exhibited reduced contact time compared with mice exposed to an anesthetized conspecific on the first day. The presence or smell of a predator provokes immediate innate fear in rodents such as rats and mice. Mice avoid the location of the predator, even when they have never been exposed to it and have no prior learning experience [40]. However, on the second and third days, mice exposed to the cadaveric conspecific displayed reduced contact duration compared with mice exposed to an anesthetized conspecific. Thus, initially the dead body was likely a novelty, and source of curiosity, rather than a source of innate fear. Unlike exposure to an anesthetized mouse, the odor from a dead body can cause increased level of anxiety and locomotion and reduced contact duration.
Although difference was not observed in the contact duration between the two groups on the first day, mice might have suspected a problem with the cadaver and acquired some information regarding the cadaver during the first investigation; they exhibited different neural activities in the mPFC when investigating a cadaveric conspecific or an anesthetized conspecific in the electrophysiological experiment. The mice may have learned to fear or experienced anxiety from repeated exposure to cadaveric conspecifics on the second and third days. However, the mice may simply have lost interest more quickly in a body that was dead compared with an alive but anesthetized body. In addition, whether the fear of a cadaveric conspecific in this study included fear of mortality could not be determined [41,42]. Further experiments are needed to determine whether exposure to a cadaveric conspecific or exposure to potential death is recognized as a fearful situation or source of anxiety in mice. In addition, further studies using familiar mice, such as cagemates or siblings, as cadavers may significantly advance the understanding of the neural basis of death-related behavior in animals.
In the present study, several experiments were performed to determine how exposure to a cadaveric conspecific affects laboratory mice and what relevance it has. Notably, exposure to the cadaveric conspecific rendered laboratory mice fearful or anxious, and changed their brain activities. Encountering the dead body of a conspecific may be a traumatic experience for laboratory mice and they may acquire fear of a cadaver without experiencing an innate fear of death. Finally, the results indicated that laboratory mice can learn about death through fear or anxiety induced by exposure to a cadaveric conspecific. Although whether laboratory mice learned or became aware of death through investigation of a cadaver conspecific is unknown, experiments using animal cadaveric conspecifics may provide information and facilitate further studies of brain disorders such as suicidality and psychosocial behaviors.
Notes
Conflicts of Interest
Daejong Jeon has been an associate editor of Encephalitis since October 2020. Sang Kun Lee and Kon Chu have been on the editorial board of Encephalitis since October 2020. They were not involved in the review process of this original article. No other potential conflict of interest relevant to this article was reported.
Author Contributions
Conceptualization, Methodology: Jeon D, Lee SK, Chu K; Investigation, Data curation, Formal analysis: Jeon D, Kim S; Resources: Lee SK, Chu K; Funding acquisition: Jeon D, Lee SK, Chu K; Writing–original draft: Jeon D; Writing–review and editing: all authors.
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank Ah reum Yang (Laboratory for Neurotherapeutics, Department of Neurology, Biomedical Research Institute, Seoul National University Hospital, Seoul, Korea) for her help in the behavioral tests. This work was supported by Korea Health 21 R&D grants (HI12C0035) funded by Ministry of Health and Welfare and supported by a grant from Advanced Neural Technologies (0620182930).